Ph Vivian
February 2018
INTRODUCTION
In accordance
with Nunan (2015, p.77), “[l]ike reading, writing is not only a tool for
communication but also an instrument for intellectual growth and development”.
However, in his recent published book, Harmer (2015, p.360) asserts that
writing is also the skill that ‘teachers and learners seem most reluctant to
focus on because it requires them to make special efforts’. Undoubtedly,
writing is a ‘complex integrated activity’ (Leggette, Rutherford, &
Dunsford, 2015, p.250) that serves as one of the prominent skills for students
in second language learning. It is, therefore, understandable that most students
find writing skills arduous and challenging.
It is obvious
that to become effective writers in the field of second language learning,
students not only need to know well WHAT they are writing, WHO they are writing
to, but also do they need to realize WHY to write and HOW to write. In my
particular teaching context, together with some genuine dilemmas like lacking
of vocabulary and collocations (lexical sources) to properly express specific
ideas, making grammatical mistakes, or translating word by word from the first
language to the second one, it is perceived that in terms of process-oriented
writing approach, students are dealing with a large number of difficulties in identifying
what to write, the message’s audience, the objectives of the text, organizing
ideas and linking the ideas in a systematic order that makes sense.
On the other
hand, recent research in second language teaching and learning has witnessed an
increasingly popular interest in learners’ metacognitive awareness and its role
in the students’ language development in terms of receptive skills such as
listening and reading (e.g. Baker & Brown, 1984; Vandergrift, 2002; Collins
& Smith, 1982; Wenden, 1998, to name just a few). It is believed that
metacognitive thinkers change both their comprehension and their strategies in
language learning development. Also, metacognitive aware learners are asserted
to be able to monitor and control the effectiveness of their learning. Bransford,
Brown, and Cocking (2000), therefore, recommend that “the teaching of
metacognitive skills should be integrated into the curriculum in a variety of
subject areas” (p.21). In the same vein, Ambrose, Bridges, DiPietro, Lovett,
and Norman (2010) believe metacognition to be one of the seven research-based principles
for smart teaching. In the new perspective, however, few writers have been able
to draw on any systematic quantitative research into the correlation between metacognition
and students’ writing proficiency, especially in the context of Vietnam. The
purpose of this paper, therefore, is to review recent research into the effects
metacognitive awareness has on students’ writing performance in the hope that to
some extent, the problems would be properly and effectively tackled in the
context of Vietnam. In addition, the paper also aims to pinpoint the great
significance and the necessity of conducting both quantitative and qualitative research
on the efficacy of metacognitive acknowledgement on Vietnamese students’
writing proficiency. Accordingly, this project provides an important
opportunity to advance the understanding of metacognition on the subject of
writing skills.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Writing as one of the prominent skills in second
language learning
Although recent related research
has revealed that writing has often been considered as an output of language
acquisition, rather than a facilitating strand, it is one of the prominent
skills which ‘must be taught and practised’ (Parel & Jain, 2008, p. 125) in
second language learning as a means of language proficiency reflection. This is
simply because in essence, it is ‘a kind of linguistic behaviour’ (Parel &
Jain, op.cit.). As stated before in this paper, as writing is a ‘complex
integrated activity’ (Leggette, Rutherford, & Dunsford, op.cit.), it
requires great effort from both teachers and students in English teaching and
learning. On account of the more traditional perspective of writing – learning-to-write
(see Hyland, 2011), it is generally the last skill to be learnt. Accordingly, it
provides a very good means of consolidating vocabulary, spelling, and sentence
patterns, hence in a long run, writing becomes an important aspect of students’
expression at higher stages (Parel & Jain, op.cit.).
Metacognition and metacognitive knowledge
The term ‘metacognition’ was
coined by Flavell in the late of 1970s. Metacognition can be briefly described
as “cognition about cognition”, “thinking about thinking” (Flavell, 1976,
p.232). In other words, it is the “refer[ence] to one’s knowledge concerning
one’s own cognitive processes or any related to them” (Flavell, op.cit.). He
has also preliminarily defined that metacognition is one’s acknowledgement of his
own thinking process and the ability to manage the cognitive processes. In the
same vein, Zimmerman (2002) maintains that ‘students’ deficiencies in learning
were attributed to a lack of metacognitive awareness of personal limitations
and an inability to compensate’. Tobias and Everson (2009) tell apart the
difference between students with developed and underdeveloped metacognition. As
regard the former, Tobias and Everson (op.cit.) claim that they ‘have an
advantage during instruction because they can omit or skim the more familiar
materials, and concentrate more fully on less familiar content they have yet to
master’. Whereas, the latter ‘with underdeveloped metacognition, i.e., those
with less accurate knowledge monitoring skills, often spend too much time
reviewing familiar material at the expense of mastering the unfamiliar or new
material and, as a consequence, often fall behind the instructional sequence’.
Metacognitive knowledge is further
developed in Flavell (1979), he enhances that metacognitive knowledge involves three
particularly striking and reciprocally interactive components: person
knowledge, task knowledge, and strategic knowledge.
As regards person knowledge, Flavell
(op.cit.) opines that it comprehends ‘everything that you could come to believe
about the nature of yourself and other people as cognitive processors. It can
be further subcategorized into beliefs about intraindividual differences,
interindividual differences, and universals of cognition’. Wenden (op.cit)
claims that person knowledge may consist of cognitive and affective factors
such as age, language aptitude, motivation, self-efficacy beliefs about their
general ability as learners, and beliefs about their ability to achieve
specific learning goals. As per writing, the above indicated factors may refer
to knowledge that the learners have obtained about themselves as writers,
including their attitude towards, motivation in, and beliefs about their
writing competence as well as their perceived capability in achieving specific
writing objectives.
With respect to task knowledge, Flavel
(op.cit.) describes that “[t]he metacognitive knowledge in this subcategory is
an understanding of what such variations imply for how the cognitive enterprise
should best be managed and how successful you are likely to be in achieving its
goal”. To be more particular, Breen (1987) justifies that task knowledge is the
students’ knowledge about the task purpose and how the task will meet the
specific needs and goals. Wenden (op.cit) also adds that it is also about the
nature of a certain task and the task’s demands such as the procedure or
technique (or approach) as well as the particular knowledge and skills to fulfill
the task. On the subject of writing, task knowledge may consist of the
students’ knowledge about the nature of a specific writing task (i.e. to
improve their writing skills), and the skills needed to complete the task such
as using proper vocabulary, accurate grammar, logically organizing ideas with
clarity.
Apropos of strategic knowledge, Flavell
(op.cit.) asserts that “there is a great deal of knowledge that could be
acquired concerning what strategies are likely to be effective in achieving
what subgoals and goals in what sorts of cognitive undertakings”. In other words,
strategic knowledge pertains to a specific effective strategy employed in order
to accomplish the short-term objectives as well as long-term aims of the required
tasks. Wenden (1998: p.519) maintains the stance by adding that metacognitive
strategies “are general skills through which learners manage, direct, regulate,
guide their learning, i.e. planning, monitoring and evaluating”. With reference
to writing, strategic knowledge is about the learners’ knowledge of writing
procedure such as pre-, while-, and post-writing during the process of writing.
In the light of Flavel’s (op.cit.)
assertion, metacognition in writing is categorized according to two major
elements: knowledge about cognition and the conscious regulation of writing
activity. (e.g. Schraw & Dennison, 1994; Schraw, 1998; Sperling, Howard,
Staley, & DuBois, 2004;; Dunlosky & Metcalfe, 2009; Serra &
Metcalfe, 2009 – cited in Negretti, 2012). As mentioned in Harris, Graham,
Brindle, and Sandmel (2009), knowledge about cognition includes three domains: declarative
knowledge, procedural knowledge, and conditional knowledge. In agreement with Harris
(et. al., op.cit.), Negretti (op.cit.) also states that declarative knowledge
refers to recognition of what strategies and concepts are important in
connection with a particular task, procedural knowledge pertains to recognition
of ‘how to apply concepts and strategies’ (how to perform the task), and conditional
knowledge appertains acknowledgement of when and why to apply specific
knowledge and strategies.
Furthermore, the
conscious regulation of writing activity is the sophisticated one for ‘skilled
writers[,] as they seek to regulate above indicated factors as selection and
use of strategies and skills, the writing environment, managing cognitive load,
their affective responses to writing, and attentional control’ (Harris et. al.,
op.cit.). By the same token, Negretti (op.cit.) has indicated that ‘metacognition
is often discussed together with self-regulation and self-regulated learning,
indicating the complex set of abilities employed by people to control their behavior
and their learning to reach desirable goals’. Metacognitive acknowledgement in writing is,
therefore, considered as ‘one of the facilitating factors’ that helps people
‘transfer skills, knowledge, and strategies across contexts and situations’
(Negretti, op.cit). Three conventional essential skills being mentioned in
relation to regulation of cognition are planning, monitoring, and evaluation
(Jacobs and Paris, 1987). As far as the researcher is concerned, a full
discussion of self-regulation and self-regulated learning lies beyond the scope
of the study.
Previous studies on metacognition
and students’ writing development
Recent
investigators have examined the effects of metacognition cognizance on
students’ writing development. Research has consistently shown that metacognitive
awareness has drawn on some positively extensive modification in student’s
writing competence.
Yanyan (2010)
conducts an investigation on the role of metacognitive knowledge in the English
writing of Chinese EFL learners. The research data in this study were drawn
from 120 freshmen who were asked to complete an English writing task and a
self-designed questionnaire on metacognition knowledge. The students’ English level
was indicated by English Proficiency Test, which contained listening
comprehension, vocabulary and grammar, reading comprehension, translation and
writing and was graded by English teachers on the ground of the same criteria, at
the end of the first semester. The research was carried out at regular English
teaching hours with the assistance of the participant’s teachers. The
metacognitive questionnaire was delivered after the English writing task was
assigned in order to make sure it would not have any impacts on students’
writing process. The documents from the students’ English compositions on the
topic “Part-time job’ were graded independently by two experienced English
teachers, following the same grading criteria as in CET-4. Additionally, the
inter-rater reliability was measured and it was over 0.9. After analyzing, the
finding of the study is that while the students’ metacognitive knowledge
foundation is not strong, all the components of metacognition knowledge are
positively correlated with English writing performance, and successful
engagement of metacognitive knowledge facilitates EFL learners’ writing
proficiency. The finding of the study suggests that ‘good command of
metacognitive knowledge can empower EFL learners in their English writing and
cultivate their learning autonomy in English learning’.
While Yanyan
(op.cit.) conducts an investigation on the role of metacognitive knowledge in
the English writing of Chinese EFL learners, Ruan (2013) has taken a further
step when examining the effects of metacognitive awareness on students’ writing
development in the context of China. Under the threefold metacognition
framework – person, task, and strategy variables, the data for this study were
collected in a Chinese tertiary English Language Teaching context through
small-group interviews with 51 English-majored students before taking an
English Writing Course. The findings of the study show that ‘motivation,
self-efficacy, and writing anxiety constitute students’ recognition of person
variables’ which in turn influences their EFL writing ‘whereas their task
awareness involves task purposes, task constraints, and cross-language task
interference’. Strategies awareness of planning, text generating, and revising was
found typical in experienced EFL student writers. The paper also proposes an
interactional model of EFL student writers’ metacognitive realization about EFL
students’ writing.
An
international model of metacognitive awareness
about EFL
writing (Adapted from Ruan, 2013)
As a result, the
finding of the study agrees with Victori (1999) that ‘successful and
unsuccessful EFL writers can be distinguished according to the adequacy of
metacognitive awareness’.
Furthermore, in the
study which set out to clarify the relationship between metacognition and ESL writing
performance by collecting and analyzing both statistical and protocol data,
Kasper (1997) reports that ‘there is a significant positive correlation between
ESL students’ metacognitive growth, along and across the three components of
metacognitive knowledge base, and their actual performance on a final writing
assessment’ (Kasper, op.cit., p. 12). As a consequence, it is safe to state
that ‘students who were successful on the final writing assessment obtained
higher ratings on all three metacognitive variables’ (Kasper, op.cit., p. 12). To
be more specific, Kasper (op.cit.) asserts that in terms of the three
individual components of the metacognitive model for writing, at least for the
120 students taking part in the study, personal and task knowledge did not
significantly changed as students progressed from the intermediate to the
advance level. On the other hand, ‘their knowledge of effective writing
strategies did increase significantly as students became more proficient in the
English language’ (p.12). The study also replicated some previous studies,
especially Brown (1980) when in his paper, he states that “metacognitive
deficiencies are a function of inexperience” (Brown, op.cit., p.475).
Overall, these
studies highlight the need to investigate the specific level of positive correlation
between metacognitive awareness and writing competence towards some particular
contexts, especially Vietnam. Also, these studies may shed lights on finding
out discrete resolutions to tackling the aforementioned problems as well as developing
EFL students’ writing ability.
Suggested metacognitive instructions
In light of the positive
correlation between metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive awareness and EFL
students’ writing competence in literature, there was an increasingly research
concerning about the efficacy of applying metacognition in EFL writing
instruction. One typical research of this kind is Xiao (2007). Xiao’s study is not
just at the constraint of the effectiveness of adopting metacognition in
students’ writing, he also propose three guidelines for EFL instructors to
‘make metacognitive teaching an integral part of writing instruction’ (Xiao,
op.cit.) on the ground of the literature in the field. Also, the paper
advocates both teaching with metacognition and teaching for metacognition. As
per the first (with), he suggests that ‘EFL writing instructor should reflect
upon his or her own teaching, and possess both metacognitive strategic
knowledge and executive management strategies’ (Xiao, op.cit., p. 23). This
means ‘teachers know about and think about their own thinking concerning their
teaching’ (p. 23). With reference to the second (for), ‘students can learn
about what the strategies are, how to use the strategies, when and why to apply
the strategies, and as the result, learn to regulate their cognitive
activities’ (Xiao, op.cit., p. 23). This means ‘teachers need to design their
own instructions that will activate and develop their students’ metacognition’
(p. 23).
Xiao (op.cit) has also explained the
reasons for proposing guidelines in relation to the issue. Firstly, ‘one
general guideline may sometimes involve more than one activity and strategy’
(Xiao, op.cit., p. 23). Secondly, ‘a creative teacher can design and develop
more and new activities by following the guideline without being restricted by
a limited set of activities’ (Xiao, op.cit., p. 24). Accordingly, his three
proposed guidelines are presented as followed:
- Guideline 1: Explicit instruction
In agreement with Paris and
Winograd (1990), Blakey & Spence (1990), Xiao (op.cit.) recommend direct
instruction as one effective classroom practice that will facilitate students to
improve their metacognitive awareness. Paris and Winograd (op.cit, p.32-33)
summarize five key features which teachers should put more focus on their
instructions in the attempt of teaching students how to learn metacognitively:
1.
What the
strategy is.
2.
Why the strategy
should be learned.
3.
How to use the
strategy.
4.
When and where
the strategy is to be used.
5.
How to evaluate
the use of the strategy.
The first four features refer
primarily the three domains of metacognitive knowledge (declarative,
procedural, and conditional knowledge) and the last feature is about the
regulation of one’s cognition (which is not the focus of this paper). The
ultimate justification of utilizing direct instruction is to ‘provide explicit
explanations on the notion and construct of metacognition’ (Xiao, op.cit., p.
26). By doing this, the students ‘who used to be subconsciously aware of or
most of the time unaware of their own cognitive activities will become
metacognitively aware of their mental actions when they perform cognitive
tasks’ (Xiao, op.cit, p.26).
- Guideline 2: Scaffolded instruction
Regarding scaffolded instruction, think sheet and cue card can be considered
as facilitators and ‘adopted by teachers to guide students in order to gradually
develop their own regulatory strategies’ (Xiao, op.cit., p. 26). this external
support is offered to the students until they are ready, in other words, until
‘their metacognitive strategies move toward an automatic state’ (Xiao, op.cit.,
p26 ). The study has also mentioned that ‘scaffolding is based on the concept
of the zone of proximal development (see Vygotsky’s, 1978 for further
understanding). ‘In other words, scaffolding involves providing support to
students to bridge the gap between what they can do on their own and what they
can do with guidance from more competent others including teachers and peers’
(Xiao, op.cit., p. 27).
In agreement with Resenshine and Meister (1992), Xiao restates six
basic guidelines for the teacher planning to practice scaffolded instructions
which are as followed:
Resenshine and Meister (1992) identified six basic
guidelines for the teachers planning to practice scaffolded instruction: (1)
present new cognitive strategies, (2) regulate any difficulties during guided
practice, (3) provide varying contexts for students to practice, (4) provide
feedback, (5) increase student responsibility, and (6) provide independent
practice. Accordingly, at the beginning of teaching students how to perform a
new task, the teacher needs to firstly model how to do it to provide the
students with complete guidance. The students observe the teacher, an expert
model, and do little independent thinking at this point. Afterwards, the
teacher provides guided practice in different contexts for the students to
practice the strategies modeled in the first step. At this stage, the students
attempt to perform the task with the support supplied by the teacher. The
support can include the teacher providing additional modeling or thinking aloud,
offering hints and feedback, and giving partial solutions. As more guided practice
is conducted, the teacher gradually transfers the responsibility to the students
by decreasing the amount of support and increasing the students’ independent
thinking. That is, the teacher’s role changes from model to facilitator, and
the practice changes from teacher’s control to students’ self-regulation.
Finally, when the strategies are internalized, the students are able to perform
the task on their own.
- Guideline 3: A school year’s training
It is suggested that metacognition
should be an integral part of the instructional objectives and accordingly, it
should be taught in the English curriculum over the school year. Xiao (op.cit.,
p. 27) affirms that “the most effective way for EFL writing teachers to teacher
their students to become metacognitive learners is probably to allow
metacognitive instruction to permeate their curriculum”. However, it is the
fact that in order to successfully adapt metacognitive instruction over the
school year, both teachers and students should also be persistent through ‘not
just a single class or unit’. In other words, the process of applying metacognitive
instructions in order to develop students’ metacognitive awareness in terms of improving
writing proficiency is time-consuming and the results may be diverse in
different teaching contexts, with varying students’ levels and due to other various
external factors.
CONCLUSION
All things considered, the present
paper has presented the specific problems the researcher is facing in the individual
teaching context. To be more precise, the researcher’s students are dealing
with some dilemmas in indicating what to write, who to write to, why to write
and how to be successful in English writing. Also, in the literature, there has
been an increasingly popular concern about how and to what extent metacognition
has positive effects on students’ writing proficiency. The present study has described
some particular earlier studies revealing the positive correlation between
metacognitive awareness and students’ writing development. It is due to the
constraint of the paper, only three previous studies in the field have been
mentioned. However, it is believed that this project has provided an important
opportunity to advance the understanding of the relationship of metacognition
and writing skill and metacognitive instruction may play a critical important role
in ameliorating students’ writing competence. Therefore, applying metacognitive
instructions in teaching writing is expected to address the indicated problems in
the researcher’s personal teaching context in particular and in others’
teaching contexts where the same writing difficulties are encountered in
general. Accordingly, future research should take metacognition, metacognitive
awareness, and metacognitive strategies into consideration in terms of enhancing
students’ writing ability, especially in the particular contexts of Vietnam. Hopefully,
this attempt in turn gets researchers, experts, and teachers’ concern about and
responsibility for proposing, assessing, and adjusting possible solutions in teaching
English writing metacognitively in Vietnam’s educational settings.
REFERENCES
[1]
Ambrose, S. A.,
Bridges, M. W., DiPietro, M., Lovett, M. C., & Norman, M. K. (2010). How
Learning Works: Seven Research-Based Principles for Smart Teaching. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.
[2]
Baker, L., &
Brown, A. L (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P. D. Pearson (Ed.). Handbook
of reading research, 254-394. NY: Longman.
[3]
Blakey, E.,
& Spence, S. (1990). Developing metacognition. ERIC Digest (ED327218).
Retrieved on February 3, 2018 from
https://www.ericdigests.org/pre-9218/developing.htm.
[4]
Bransford, J.
D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, A. R. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind,
experience, and school. Retrieved on January 27, 2018 from https://www.nap.edu/read/9853/chapter/1.
[5]
Breen, M. P.
(1987). Learner contributions to task design. In C. N. Candlin, & D.
Murphy, (Ed.). Language learning tasks:
Lancaster practical papers in English language education. London, UK:
Prentice Hall International.
[6]
Brown, A. L.
(1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce,
& W. F. Brewer (Eds). Theoretical issues in reading comprehension, 453-481.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[7]
Collins, A.,
& Smith, E. E. (1982). Teaching the process of reading comprehension. In
D.K. Detterman & R.J. Sternberg (Eds.). How and how much can intelligence
be increased, 13-185. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
[8]
Flavell, J. H.
(1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L.B. Resnick (Ed). The
nature of intelligence. 231-236. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
[9]
Flavell, J. H.
(1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of
cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911. Retrieved
on January 27, 2018 from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232599909_Metacognition_and_Cognitive_Monitoring_A_New_Area_of_Cognitive-Developmental_Inquiry.
[10]
Harmer, J.
(2015). The Practice of English Language Teaching. 5th Edition. London:
Pearson.
[11]
Harris, K. R.,
Graham, S., Brindle, M., & Sandmel, K. (2009). Metacognition and Children’s
Writing. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.). Handbook of
Metacognition in Education, 131-153. NY: Routledge. Retrieved on January 27,
2018 from https://zodml.org/sites/default/files/%5BDouglas_J._Hacker,_John_Dunlosky,_Arthur_C._Graes_0.pdf.
[12]
Hyland, K.
(2011). Learning to write: Issues in Theory, research and pedagogy. In R. M.
Manchón, (Ed.). Learning-to-write and writing-to-learn in an additional
language, 17-35. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
[13]
Jacobs, J. E.
& Paris, S. G. (1987). Children’s metacognition about reading: Issues in definition,
measurement, and instruction. Educ. Psychol, 22, 255-278.
[14]
Kasper, L. F.
(1997). Assessing the Metacognitive Growth of ESL Students Writers. The
Electronic Journal for English as a Second Language, 3(1), 1-20.
[15]
Leggette, H. R.,
Rutherford, T. A., & Dunsford, D. W. (2015). A Model to Augment Critical
Thinking and Creative Knowledge through Writing in the Agricultural Social
Sciences. NACTA Journal, 59(3), 245-251. Retrieved from http://nactateachers.org/index.php/volume-59-number-3-september-2015.
[16]
Nunan, D. (2015).
Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages – An Introduction. NY:
Routledge.
[17]
Negretti, R.
(2012). Metacognition in student academic writing: A longitudinal study of
metacognitive awareness and its relation to task perception, self-regulation, and
evaluation of performance. Written Communication, 29(2), 142-179.
[18]
Paris, S. G.,
& Winograd, P. (1990). How metacognition can promote academic learning and
instruction. In B. F. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.). Dimensions of thinking and
cognitive instruction, 15-51. Hilldales, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[19]
Patel, M. F.
& Jain, P. M. (2008). English Language Teaching (Methods, Tools,
Techniques). Vaishali Nagar: Jaipur.
[20]
Tobias, S.,
Everson, H. T. (2009). The importance of knowing what you know. In D. J.
Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.). Handbook of Metacognition in
Education, 131-153. NY: Routledge. Retrieved on January 27, 2018 from https://zodml.org/sites/default/files/%5BDouglas_J._Hacker,_John_Dunlosky,_Arthur_C._Graes_0.pdf
[21]
Rosenshine, B.,
& Meister, C. (1992). The use of scaffolds for teaching higher-level
cognitive strategies. Educational Leadership, April, 26-33.
[22]
Ruan, Z. (2013).
Metacognitive Awareness of EFL Student Writers in a Chinese ELT Context. Language
Awareness, 23(1-2), 76-90.
[23]
Vandergrift, L.
(2002). It is nice to see that out predictions were right: Developing
metacognition in L2 listening comprehension. The Canadian Modern Language
Review, 58, 555-575.
[24]
Victori. M.
(1999). An analysis of writing knowledge in EFL composing: A case study of two
effective and two less effective writers. System, 27(4), 537-556.
[25]
Vygotsky, L.
(1978). Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
[26]
Wenden, A. L.
(1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language learning. Applied Linguistics, 19(4),
515-537. Retrieved from https://academic.oup.com/applij/article-abstract/19/4/515/265646?redirectedFrom=PDF.
[27]
Xiao, Y. (2007).
Applying metacognition in EFL writing instruction in China. Reflection on
English Language Teaching, 6(1), 19-33. Retrieved from http://www.researchgate.net/publication/253991425.
[28]
Yanyan, Z.
(2010). Investigating the Role of Metacognitive Knowledge in English Writing. HKBU
Papers in Applied Language Studies, 14, 25-46.
[29]
Zimmerman, B. J.
(2002). Becoming a Self-regulated Learner: An Overview. THEORY INTO PRACTICE,
41(2), 63-70. Retrieved on January 30, 2018 from http://mathedseminar.pbworks.com/w/file/fetch/94760840/Zimmerman%20-%202002%20-%20Becoming%20a%20Self-Regulated%20Learner%20An%20Overview.pdf.
No comments:
Post a Comment